By F. HOLMES ATWATER The Monroe Institute, 62 Roberts Mountain Round, Fuber, VA 22938-2317 Journal of Scientific Exploration Vol 1, No 3, 1997 |
Abstract: |
Exposure to binaural beats in an environment of restricted stimulation
coupled with a guidance process can safely provide access to and experiences
in many propitious states of consciousness. This method requires a
unique combination of well-understood psycho-physiological inductive
techniques with the addition of a refined binaural-beat technology. Binaural
beats provide potential consciousness-altering information to the brain's
reticular activating system. The reticular activating system in turn interprets
and reacts to this information by stimulating the thalamus and cortex -
thereby altering arousal states, attentional focus, and the level of awareness,
i.e., the elements of consciousness itself. This effective binaural-beat
process offers a wide variety of beneficial applications and vehicle for the exploration
of expanded states of consciousness.
Keywords: consciousness - altered states, brainwave entrainment, isochronic tones, monaural tones |
Introduction: |
The audio phenomenon known as binaural beating can be used to access altered states of consciousness. This is done through a process in which individuals in an environment of restricted stimulation willfully focus attention on a combination of multiplexed audio binaural beats that are mixed with music, pink sound', and/or assorted natural sounds. In most cases the process also includes breathing exercises, guided relaxation, affirmation, and visualization. The binaural-beat element of the process appears to be associated with an electroencephalographic (EEG) frequency-following response in the brain.2 Many studies have demonstrated the presence of a frequency-following response to auditory stimuli, recorded at the vertex of the human brain (top of the head). This EEG activity was termed "frequency-following response" because its period (cycles per second) corresponds to the fundamental frequency of the stimulus (Smith, Marsh, and Brown, 1975). Stated plainly, if the audio stimulus is 40 Hz the resulting measured EEG will show a 40 Hz frequency-following response using appropriate time-domain averaging protocols. Binaural- beat stimulation, coupled with the effects of the other procedures within the process outlined above, appears to regulate neuronal activity and encourage access to propitious mental states. The effectiveness of binaural beats in engendering state changes is supported by the consistent reports of thousands of users, as well as the documentation of physiological changes associated with its use. The reported uses of this binaural-beat method for accessing propitious states of consciousness range from sensory integration (Morris, 1990), relaxation, meditation, stress reduction, pain management, improved sleep (Wilson, 1990; Rhodes, 1993), health care (Carter, 1993), enriched learning environments and enhanced memory (Kennerly, 1994) to creativity (Hiew, 1995), how to attain siddhi powers and abilities, enhanced intuition, psychic development, remote viewing3 (McMoneagle, 1993), telepathy4, out-of-body experience, and other assorted siddhi prapti mantra supernatural powers. An understanding of the applied binaural-beat technology involves the well- known autonomic effects of controlled breathing and progressive relaxation and the psychology of affirmations and visualizations (subjects not addressed in this paper). For the purposes of this paper, discussion is limited to the physiology of the brain, the brain-mind model, brain waves and their relationship to the behavioral psychology of consciousness, and the role of the reticular activating system (RAS) in regulating brain waves and consciousness.
|
Binaural Beats and the Physiology of the Brain: |
The sensation of "hearing" binaural beats occurs when two coherent sounds of nearly similar frequencies are presented, one to each ear, and the brain detects phase differences between these sounds. This phase difference normally provides directional information to the listener, but when presented with stereo headphones or speakers the brain integrates the two signals, producing a sensation of a third sound called the binaural beat. Perceived as a fluctuating rhythm at the frequency of the difference between the two (stereo left and right) auditory inputs, binaural beats appear to originate in the brain stem's superior olivary nucleus, the site of contralateral integration of auditory input (Oster, 1973). This auditory sensation is neurologically routed to the reticular formation (Swann et al., 1982) and simultaneously volume conducted to the cortex where it can be objectively measured as a frequency-following response (Oster, 1973; Smith, Marsh, and Brown, 1975; Marsh, Brown, and Smith, 1975; Smith et al., 1978; Hink et al., 1980). The frequency-following response provides proof that the sensation of binaural beating has neurological efficacy. Binaural beats can easily be heard at the low frequencies (< 30 Hz) that are characteristic of the EEG spectrum (Oster, 1973; Atwater, 1997). This perceptual phenomenon of binaural beating and the objective measurement of the frequency-following response (Oster, 1973; Hink et al., 1980) suggest conditions which facilitate alteration of brain waves and states of consciousness. There have been numerous anecdotal reports and a growing number of research efforts reporting changes in consciousness associated with binaural and monauralbeats. Binaural beats and isochronic tones in the delta (1 to 4 Hz) and theta (4 to 8 Hz) ranges have been associated with reports of relaxed, meditative, and creative states (Hiew, 1995), sensory integration (Morris, 1990), and used as an aid to falling asleep (Wilson, 1990; Rhodes, 1993). Exposure to audio-guidance training using lower-frequency binaural beats in concert with cognitive therapy resulted in decreased depressive symptoms in alcoholic patients (Waldkoetter & Sanders, 1997). Binaural beats in the alpha frequencies (8 to 12 Hz) have increased alpha brain waves (Foster, 1990) and binaural beats in the beta frequencies (typically 16 to 24 Hz) have been associated with reports of increased concentration or alertness (Monroe, 1985), improved memory (Kennerly, 1994), and increases in focused attention in mentally retarded adults (Guilfoyle & Carbone, 1996). Passively listening to binaural beats may not automatically engender an altered state of consciousness. The process usually used when listening to binaural beats includes a number of procedures; binaural beats are only one element. We all maintain a psycho-physiological momentum, a homeostasis which may resist the influence of the binaural beats. These homeostatic states are generally controlled by life situations as well as by acts of will, both conscious and subconscious. The willingness and ability of the listener to relax and focus attention or their level of practice in meditative processes may in some way contribute to binaural-beat effectiveness. Naturally occurring neurological ultradian rhythms, characterized by periodic changes in arousal and states of consciousness (Webb & Dube, 198 1 ; Rossi, 1986; Shannahoff-Khalsa, 1991), may underlie the anecdotal reports of fluctuations in the effectiveness of binaural beats. The perception of a binaural beat is said to be heightened by the addition of masking noise to the carrier signal (Oster, 1973), so white or pink noise is often used as background. Practices such as humming, toning, breathing exercises, autogenic training, and/or biofeedback can also be used to interrupt the homeostasis of subjects resistant to the effects of binaural beats (Tart, 1975). |
Brain Waves and Consciousness: |
Controversies concerning the brain, mind, and consciousness have existed since the early Greek philosophers argued about the nature of the mind-body relationship, and none of these disputes have been resolved. Modern neurologists have located the mind in the brain and have said that consciousness is the result of electrochemical neurological activity. There are, however, growing observations challenging the completeness of these assertions. There is no neuro-physiological research which conclusively shows that the higher levels of mind (intuition, insight, creativity, imagination, understanding, thought, reasoning, intent, decision, knowing, will, spirit, or soul) are located in brain tissue (Hunt, 1995). A resolution to the controversies surrounding the higher mind and consciousness and the mind-body problem in general may need to involve an epistemological shift to include extra-rational ways of knowing (de Quincey, 1994) and may well not be comprehended by neuro-chemical brain studies alone. Penfield (1 975), an eminent contemporary neuro-physiologist, found that the human mind continued to work in spite of the brain's reduced activity under anesthesia. Brain waves were nearly absent while the mind was just as active as in the waking state. The only difference was in the content of the conscious experience. Following Penfield's work, other researchers have reported awareness in comatose patients (Hunt, 1995), and there is a growing body of evidence which suggests that reduced cortical arousal while maintaining conscious awareness is possible (Fischer, 197 1 ; West, 1980; Delmonte, 1984; Wallace, 1986; Goleman, 1988; Mavromatis, 199 1 ; Jevning, Wallace, and Beidenbach, 1992). These states are variously referred to as meditative, trance, altered, hypnagogic, hypnotic, and twilight-learning states (Budzynski, 1986). Broadly defined, the various forms of altered states rest on the maintenance of conscious awareness in a physiologically reduced state of arousal marked by parasympathetic dominance (Mavromatis, 199 1). Recent physiological studies of highly hypnotizable subjects and adept meditators indicate that maintaining awareness with reduced cortical arousal is indeed possible in selected individuals as a natural ability or as an acquired skill (Sabourin, Cutcomb, Crawford, and Pribram, 1993). More and more scientists are expressing doubts about the neurologists' brain-mind model because it fails to answer so many questions about our ordinary experiences, as well as evading our mystical and spiritual ones. Studies in distant mental influence and mental healing also challenge the notion of a mind localized within the brain (Dossey, 1994, 1996a). Nonlocal events have been proven to occur at the subatomic level, and some researchers believe that the physics principles behind these events underlie nonlocal consciousness-mediated effects (Dossey, 1996a). consciousness - associated anomalies appear unrestricted by spatial or temporal boundaries and many experiments have been done to shed light on this remarkable quality of the mind (Dossey, 1996b). The scientific evidence supporting the phenomenon of remote viewing alone is sufficient to show that mind-consciousness is not a local phenomenon (McMoneagle, 1993). If mind-consciousness is not the brain, why then does science relate states of consciousness and mental functioning to brain-wave frequencies? There is no objective way to measure mind or consciousness with an instrument. Mind consciousness appears to be a field phenomenon which interfaces with the body and the neurological structures of the brain (Hunt, 1995). One cannot measure this field directly with current instrumentation. On the other hand, the electrical potentials of the body can be measured and easily quantified. The problem here lies in oversimplification of the observations. EEG patterns measured on the cortex are the result of electro-neurological activity of the brain. But the brain's electro-neurological activity is not mind-consciousness. EEG measurements then are only an indirect means of assessing the mind-consciousness interface with the neurological structures of the brain. As crude as this may seem, the EEG has been a reliable way for researchers to estimate states of consciousness based on the relative proportions of EEG frequencies. Stated another way, certain EEG patterns have been historically associated with specific states of consciousness. Although not an absolute, it is reasonable to assume, given the current EEG literature, that if a specific EEG pattern emerges it is probably accompanied by a particular state of consciousness. Binaural beats can alter the electrochemical environment of the brain allowing mind-consciousness to have different experiences. When brain waves move to lower frequencies and awareness is maintained, a unique state of consciousness emerges. Practitioners of the binaural-beat process call this state of hypnagogia "mind awake/body asleep". Slightly higher frequencies can lead to hyper-suggestive states of consciousness. Still higher-frequency EEG states are associated with alert and focused mental activity needed for the optimal performance of many tasks. Perceived reality changes depending on the state of consciousness of the perceiver (Tart, 1975). Some states of consciousness provide limited views of reality, while others provide an expanded awareness of reality. For the most part, states of consciousness change in response to the ever-changing internal environment and surrounding stimulation. For example, states of consciousness are subject to influences like drugs and circadian and ultradian rhythms (Webb & Dube, 1981 ; Rossi, 1986; Shannahoff-Khalsa, 199 1). Specific states of consciousness can also be learned as adaptive behaviors to demanding circumstances (Green & Green, 1986). Binaural-beat technology offers access to a wide variety of altered-state experiences for those wanting to explore the realms of consciousness. |
Hemispheric Synchronization: |
Many of the states of consciousness available through this technology have been identified as presenting unique hemispherically synchronized brain-wave frequencies. Although synchronized brain waves have long been associated with meditative and hypnagogic states, the binaural-beat process may be unique in its ability to induce and improve such states of consciousness. The reason for this is physiological. Each ear is "hardwired" (so to speak) to both hemispheres of the brain (Rosenzweig, 1961). Each hemisphere has its own olivary nucleus (sound-processing center) which receives signals from each ear. In keeping with this physiological structure, when a binaural beat is perceived there are actually two electrochemical synaptic waves of equal amplitude and frequency present, one in each hemisphere. This is, in and of itself, hemispheric synchrony of synaptic activity. Binaural beats appear to contribute to the hemispheric synchronization evidenced in meditative and hypnagogic states of consciousness. Binaural beats may also enhance brain function by enabling the user to mediate cross-colossal connectivity at designated brain wave frequencies. The two cerebral hemispheres of the brain are like two separate information processing modules. Both are complex cognitive systems; both process information independently and in parallel; and their interaction is neither arbitrary nor continuous (Zaidel, 1985). Because of this, states of consciousness (mind/consciousness interfacing with the brain) can be defined not only in terms of brain-wave frequency ratios, but also in terms of hemispheric specialization and/or interaction. Some desired states of consciousness may require facile inter-hemispheric integration, while others may call for a unique hemispheric processing style. An individual's cognitive repertoire and, therefore, his ability to perceive reality and deal with the everyday world, is subject to his ability to experience various states of consciousness (Tart, 1975). Binaural beats provide the tools for individuals to expand their ability to experience a wide range of mind-consciousness states. Each state of consciousness is not represented by one simple brain wave but involves a milieu of inner-mixing wave forms, a field effect. The reason for this lies in the structure of the brain itself. Not only is the brain divided horizontally into hemispheres, it is also divided vertically from the brain stem to the cerebellum, the thalamus, the limbic system, and the cerebral cortex. The cerebral cortex is further divided into such functional areas as the frontal lobes, the parietal lobes, the temporal lobes, and the occipital lobes. There are, of course, many other subdivisions of the brain which have not been mentioned. The critical point is that for each discrete state of consciousness, mind-consciousness interfaces with each area of the brain and each area resonates at a specific brain-wave frequency unique to that interface because it performs a localized function (Luria, 1970). |
Developing Effective Binaural Beats: |
The process of developing effective stimuli relied initially on the feedback of those experiencing altered states while listening to binaural beats (Atwater, 1997), and more recently with the aid of EEG technology. Originally, researchers tested many subjects under laboratory conditions for their responses to binaural-beat stimuli. Records were kept as to the effect each binaural-beat frequency had on these subjects. Then binaural beats were mixed and records were again kept on the subjects' responses. After months (in some cases, years), test results began to show population-wide similar responses to specific mixes of binaural beats. Certain complex, brain-wave-like combinations of binaural beats were reported more effective than other combinations, and more effective than binaural beats of single frequencies (sine waves). Effective binaural beats are, therefore, unique in that they are designed to be complex brain-wave-like patterns rather than simple sine waves. (See Figures 1 and 2 Following) Figure 1. Complex Binaural Beat: 2 seconds.
Figure 2. Sine Wave Binaural Beat: 2 seconds
|
How Binaural Beats Alter States of Consciousness: |
Two decades ago it was assumed that the mechanism behind the consciousness - altering effects of binaural beats was somehow related to entrainment of the auditory frequency-following response - a theorized process of nonlinear stochastic resonance of brain waves with the frequency of the auditory stimulus. Since an auditory frequency-following response could be measured at the cortex it seemed logical to assume that the underlying consciousness-altering mechanism must be some form of Newtonian entrainment process at work. Continuing research revealed, however, that there is no effect-mechanism to support the notion that entrainment of the auditory frequency-following response could occur or is responsible for alterations in consciousness. Comparisons to photic entrainment models are not supported because the EEG signal strength of the measured auditory frequency-following response of binaural beats is too low. At this point it is hard to even speculate that the neural activity of the frequency-following response could, in some electromagnetically inductive way, alter ongoing brain-wave activity. A review of the appropriate literature reveals that brain waves and related states of consciousness are said to be regulated by the brain's reticular formation stimulating the thalamus and cortex. The extended reticular-thalamic activation system (ERTAS) is implicated in a variety of functions associated with consciousness (Newman, 1997). The word reticular means "net-like" and the neural reticular formation itself is a large, net-like diffuse area of the brainstem (Anch et al., 1988). The reticular activating system (RAS) interprets and reacts to information from internal stimuli, feelings, attitudes, and beliefs as well as external sensory stimuli by regulating arousal states, attentional focus, and the level of awareness - the elements of consciousness itself (Empson, 1986; Tice & Steinberg, 1989). How we interpret, respond, and react to information then, is managed by the brain's reticular formation stimulating the thalamus and cortex, and controlling attentiveness and level of arousal (Empson, 1986). "It would seem that the basic mechanisms underlying consciousness are closely bound up with the brainstem reticular system ..." (Henry, 1 992). In order to alter consciousness it is necessary to provide some sort of information input to the RAS. Binaural beats appear to influence consciousness by providing this information. The information referred to here includes the character, quality, and traits of the state of consciousness of the complex, brainwave- like pattern of the binaural beat (see Figure 2). These unique binauralbeat wave forms (neurologically evidenced by the EEG frequency-following response) are recognized by the RAS as brain-wave pattern information. If internal stimuli, feelings, attitudes, beliefs, and external sensory stimuli are not in conflict with this information (e.g., an internal, even unconscious, fear may be a source of conflict), the RAS will alter the state of consciousness as a natural function of maintaining homeostasis8 by regulating brain activity to synthesize the integrated binaural-beat stimulus (sensing it as a component of ongoing neural activity). Without conflict, the RAS initiates replication of the character, quality, and traits of the neurologically evident and persistent binaural beating. As time passes, the RAS monitors both the internal and external environment and the state of consciousness itself (in terms of neural activity) to determine, from moment to moment, its suitability for dealing with existing conditions. As long as no conflicts develop, the RAS naturally continues aligning the listener's state of consciousness with the information in the brain-wave-like pattern of the binaural sound field. In objective, measurable terms EEG-based research provides evidence of binaural beat's influence on consciousness. Since the RAS regulates cortical EEG (Swann, et al., 1982), monitoring EEG chronicles performance of the RAS. There have been several free-running EEG studies (Foster, 1990; Sadigh, 1990; Hiew, 1995, among others) which suggest that binaural beating induces alterations in EEG. Because the RAS is responsible for regulating EEG (Swann et al., 1982; Empson, 1986), these studies document measurable changes in RAS function during exposure to binaural beats. It is tempting to speculate about a neuro-physiological model underlying a binaural-beat-engendered state of consciousness labeled mind awake/body asleep, a hypnagogic experience common to many. In this state, a greater proportion of lower frequency brain waves (theta and delta) have been recorded in the EEG. The "body asleep" part of this state may be tied to the increase in delta waves associated with hyper-polarization of thalamocortical cells (Steriade, McCormick, and Sejnowski, 1993). The "mind awake" part of this state may be associated with theta frequencies in a portion of the hippo-campus. One is said to have achieved this state of mind-consciousness when a new condition of hypnagogic homeostasis is established and one becomes oblivious to the location of body extremities (hands, feet, etc.), still without losing consciousness (falling asleep). |
Summary: |
The binaural-beat auditory-guidance process provides access to many beneficial mind-consciousness states. This process is a unique combination of well-understood psycho-physiological inductive techniques (restricted environmental stimulation, controlled breathing, progressive relaxation, affirmation, visualization, etc.) with the addition of a refined binaural-beat technology providing potential consciousness-altering information to the brain's reticular activating system. This safe and effective binaural-beat process offers a wide variety of applications which include, but are not limited to: relaxation, meditation, enhanced creativity, intuition development, enriched learning, improved sleep, wellness, and the exploration of expanded mind-consciousness states. |
References: |
Anch, A. M., Browman, C. P., Mitler, M. M. and Walsh, J. K. (1988). Sleep: A Scientific Perspective.
Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall, p. 96-97. Budzynski, T. H. (1986). Clinical applications of non-drug-induced states. In B. B. Wolman & M.
Ullman (Eds.), Handbook of States of Consciousness, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold
Company, 428. Dossey, L. (1996a). Dialogue. Subtle Energies, 5,3,264. Smith, J. C.. Marsh, J. T., and Brown, W. S. (1975). Far-field recorded frequency-following responses:
Evidence for the locus of brainstem sources. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Neurophysiology, 39,465. |
Click here to see our Privacy Statement
This site is copyright © 2014. All rights reserved.